Frank E Pollick

home

my lab site

teaching

talks

vita

other stuff

 

Humanoids 2001 Tutorial on the Visual Perception of Human Movement

 

Talk (with references included)

Bibliography

Links

 

Selected Bibliography

Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11(2), 231-239.

Ahlstrom, V., Blake, R., & Ahlstrom, U. (1997). Perception of biological motion. Perception, 26(12), 1539-1548.

Allison, T., Puce, A., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(7), 267-278.

Barclay, C.D., Cutting, J.E. & Kozlowski, L.T. (1978). Temporal and spatial factors in gait perception that influence gender recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 23, 145-152.

Bellefeuille, A., & Faubert, J. (1998). Independence of contour and biological-motion cues for motion- defined animal shapes. Perception, 27(2), 225-235.

Bernstein, L. J., & Cooper, L. A. (1997). Direction of motion influences perceptual identification of ambiguous figures. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 23(3), 721-737.

Bertenthal, B.I. & Pinto, J. (1994). Global processing of biological motions. Psychological Science, 5, 221-225.

Bingham, G.P. (1987). Kinematic form and scaling: Further investigations on the visual perception of lifted weight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 155-177.

Bingham, G.P. (1993). Scaling judgments of lifted weight: Lifter size and the role of the standard. Ecological Psychology, 5, 31-64.

Blake, R. (1993). Cats Perceive Biological Motion. Psychological Science, 4(1), 54-57.

Blakemore, S. J., & Decety, J. (2001). From the perception of action to the understanding of intention. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(8), 561-567.

Bonda, E., Petrides, M., Ostry, D., & Evans, A. (1996). Specific involvement of human parietal systems and the amygdala in the perception of biological motion. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(11), 3737-3744.

Bulthoff, I., Bulthoff, H., & Sinha, P. (1998). Top-down influences on stereoscopic depth-perception. Nature Neuroscience, 1(3), 254-257.

Castelli, F., Happe, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: A functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage, 12(3), 314-325.

Cavanagh, P., Labianca, A. T., & Thornton, I. M. (2001). Attention-based visual routines: sprites. Cognition, 80(1-2), 47-60.

Chao, L. L., Haxby, J. V., & Martin, A. (1999). Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects. Nature Neuroscience, 2(10), 913-919.

Chatterjee, S. H., Freyd, J. J., & Shiffrar, M. (1996). Configural processing in the perception of apparent biological motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 22(4), 916-929.

Cochin, S., Barthelemy, C., Roux, S., & Martineau, J. (2001). Electroencephalographic activity during perception of motion in childhood. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13(9), 1791-1796.

Cowey, A., & Vaina, L. M. (2000). Blindness to form from motion despite intact static form perception and motion detection. Neuropsychologia, 38(5), 566-578.

Cutting, J.E. & Kozlowski, L.T. (1977). Recognizing friends by their walk: Gait perception without familiarity cues. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 353-356.

Cutting, J.E. (1978). Generation of synthetic male and female walkers through manipulation of a biomechanical invariant. Perception, 7, 393-405.

Cutting, J.E., Proffitt, D.R. & Kozlowski, L.T. (1978). A biomechanical invariant for gait perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 357-372.

Daems, A., & Verfaillie, K. (1999). Viewpoint-dependent priming effects in the perception of human actions and body postures. Visual Cognition, 6(6), 665-693.

Decety, J., & Grezes, J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human actions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(5), 172-178.

Decety, J., Grezes, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., Grassi, F., & Fazio, F. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions - Influence of action content and subject's strategy. Brain, 120, 1763-1777.

Desperati, C., & Stucchi, N. (1995). Visual Tuning to Kinematics of Biological Motion - the Role of Eye-Movements. Experimental Brain Research, 105(2), 254-260.

Dittrich, W. H. (1993). Action Categories and the Perception of Biological Motion. Perception, 22(1), 15-22.

Dittrich, W. H. (1999). Seeing biological motion - Is there a role for cognitive strategies?, Gesture-Based Communication in Human-Computer Interaction (Vol. 1739, pp. 3-22). Berlin: SPRINGER-VERLAG BERLIN.

Dittrich, W. H., Lea, S. E. G., Barrett, J., & Gurr, P. R. (1998). Categorization of natural movements by pigeons: Visual concept discrimination and biological motion. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 70(3), 281-299.

Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S. E. G., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25(6), 727-738.

Downing, P. E., Jiang, Y. H., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science, 293(5539), 2470-2473.

Dror, R. O., O'Carroll, D. C., & Laughlin, S. B. (2000). The role of natural image statistics in biological motion estimation, Biologically Motivated Computer Vision, Proceeding (Vol. 1811, pp. 492-501). Berlin: SPRINGER-VERLAG BERLIN.

Egelhaaf, M., Borst, A., & Reichardt, W. (1989). Computational Structure of a Biological Motion-Detection System as Revealed by Local Detector Analysis in the Flys Nervous- System. Journal of the Optical Society of America a-Optics Image Science and Vision, 6(7), 1070-1087.

Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The Perception of Biological Motion by Human Infants. Science, 218(4571), 486-487.

Frego, R. J. D. (1999). Effects of aural and visual conditions on response to perceived artistic tension in music and dance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47(1), 31-43.

Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Cognitive psychology - Interacting minds - A biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692-1695.

Giese, M. A., & Poggio, T. (2000). Morphable models for the analysis and synthesis of complex motion patterns. International Journal of Computer Vision, 38(1), 59-73.

Gillner, W. J. (1998). Motion segmentation in artificial and biological systems. Zeitschrift Fur Naturforschung C-a Journal of Biosciences, 53(7-8), 622-627.

Grezes, J., & Costes, N. (1998). Top-down effect of strategy on the perception of human biological motion: A PET investigation. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(6-8), 553-582.

Grezes, J., Fonlupt, P., Bertenthal, B., Delon-Martin, C., Segebarth, C., & Decety, J. (2001). Does perception of biological motion rely on specific brain regions? Neuroimage, 13(5), 775-785.

Grossman, E., Donnelly, M., Price, R., Pickens, D., Morgan, V., Neighbor, G., & Blake, R. (2000). Brain areas involved in perception of biological motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 711-720.

Grossman, E. D., & Blake, R. (1999). Perception of coherent motion, biological motion and form-from- motion under dim-light conditions. Vision Research, 39(22), 3721-3727.

Grossman, E. D., & Blake, R. (2001). Brain activity evoked by inverted and imagined biological motion. Vision Research, 41(10-11), 1475-1482.

Haken, H., Kelso, J. A. S., Fuchs, A., & Pandya, A. S. (1990). Dynamic Pattern-Recognition of Coordinated Biological Motion. Neural Networks, 3(4), 395-401.

Hermsdorfer, J., Goldenberg, G., Wachsmuth, C., Conrad, B., Ceballos-Baumann, A. O., Bartenstein, P., Schwaiger, M., & Boecker, H. (2001). Cortical correlates of gesture processing: Clues to the cerebral mechanisms underlying apraxia during the imitation of meaningless gestures. Neuroimage, 14(1), 149-161.

Hill, H., & Johnston, A. (2001). Categorizing sex and identity from the biological motion of faces. Current Biology, 11(11), 880-885.

Hill, H., & Pollick, F. E. (2000). Exaggerating temporal differences enhances recognition of individuals from point light displays. Psychological Science, 11(3), 223-228.

Hirashima, S. (1999). Recognition on the gender of point-light walkers moving in different directions. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 70(2), 149-153.

Hodgins, J. K., O'Brien, J. F., & Tumblin, J. (1998). Perception of human motion with different geometric models. Ieee Transactions on Visualization and Computer Graphics, 4(4), 307-316.

Hoenkamp, E. (1978). Perceptual cues that determine the labeling of human gait. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 4, 59-69.

Hoffman, D. D., & Flinchbaugh, B. E. (1982). The Interpretation of Biological Motion. Biological Cybernetics, 42(3), 195-204.

Ibbotson, M. R., & Clifford, C. W. G. (2001). Characterising temporal delay filters in biological motion detectors. Vision Research, 41(18), 2311-2323.

Ille, A., & Cadopi, M. (1995). Reproducing choreographic walks from the observation of a kinematic point-light display: Effect of skill level. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 29(3), 101-114.

Ishiguchi, A. (1991). Effects of Change of Spatial Distance between Moving Dots on Their Apparent Linkage. Japanese Psychological Research, 33(2), 86-96.

Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201-211.

Johansson, G. (1975). Visual motion perception. Scientific American, 232, 76-88.

Kandel, S., Orliaguet, J. P., & Viviani, P. (2000). Perceptual anticipation in handwriting: The role of implicit motor competence. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(4), 706-716.

Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Recognition of self-generated actions from kinematic displays of drawing. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 456-465.

Kourtzi, Z., & Shiffrar, M. (1999). Dynamic representations of human body movement. Perception, 28(1), 49-62.

Kozlowski, L.T. & Cutting, J.E. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception and Psychophysics, 21, 575-580.

Mather, G., & Murdoch, L. (1994). Gender Discrimination in Biological Motion Displays Based on Dynamic Cues. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 258(1353), 273-279.

Mather, G., Radford, K., & West, S. (1992). Low-Level Visual Processing of Biological Motion. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, 249(1325), 149-155.

Mather, G., & West, S. (1993). Recognition of Animal Locomotion from Dynamic Point-Light Displays. Perception, 22(7), 759-766.

McLeod, P., Dittrich, W., Driver, J., Perrett, D., & Zihl, J. (1996). Preserved and impaired detection of structure from motion by a ''motion-blind'' patient. Visual Cognition, 3(4), 363-391.

Michaels, C. F., & de Vries, M. M. (1998). Higher order and lower order variables in the visual perception of relative pulling force. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 24(2), 526-546.

Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., & Anderson, M. (1995). Person Perception - Does It Involve Iq-Independent Perceptual Processing. Intelligence, 20(1), 65-86.

Moore, D. G., Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 401-423.

Neri, P., Morrone, M. C., & Burr, D. C. (1998). Seeing biological motion. Nature, 395(6705), 894-896.

Olofsson, U., Nyberg, L., & Nilsson, L. G. (1997). Priming and recognition of human motion patterns. Visual Cognition, 4(4), 373-382.

Oram, M. W., & Perrett, D. I. (1994). Responses of Anterior Superior Temporal Polysensory (Stpa) Neurons to Biological Motion Stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(2), 99-116.

Owens, D. A., Antonoff, R. J., & Francis, E. L. (1994). Biological Motion and Nighttime Pedestrian Conspicuousness. Human Factors, 36(4), 718-732.

Pavlova, M., Krageloh-Mann, I., Sokolov, A., & Birbaumer, N. (2001). Recognition of point-light biological motion displays by young children. Perception, 30(8), 925-933.

Pavlova, M., & Sokolov, A. (2000). Orientation specificity in biological motion perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 62(5), 889-899.

Pellecchia, G. L., & Garrett, G. E. (1997). Assessing lumbar stabilization from point-light and normal video displays of manual lifting. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85(3), 931-937.

Perani, D., Fazio, F., Borghese, N. A., Tettamanti, M., Ferrari, S., Decety, J., & Gilardi, M. C. (2001). Different brain correlates for watching real and virtual hand actions. Neuroimage, 14(3), 749-758.

Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (1999). Subconfigurations of the human form in the perception of biological motion displays. Acta Psychologica, 102(2-3), 293-318.

Pollick, F.E., Sapiro, G. (1997). Constant affine velocity predicts the 1/3 power law of drawing and planar motion perception. Vision Research, 37, 347-353.

Pollick, F. E., Fidopiastis, C., & Braden, V. (2001). Recognising the style of spatially exaggerated tennis serves. Perception, 30(3), 323-338.

Pollick, F.E., Paterson, H., Bruderlin, A. & Sanford, A.J. (2001) Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82, B51--B61.

Richardson, K., & Webster, D. S. (1996). Object recognition from point-light stimuli: Evidence of covariation structures in conceptual representation. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 567-591.

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(9), 661-670.

Rosenblum, L. D., Johnson, J. A., & Saldana, H. M. (1996). Point-light facial displays enhance comprehension of speech in noise. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39(6), 1159-1170.

Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1981). Visual perception of lifted weight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 733-740.

Runeson, S., & Frykholm, G. (1983). Kinematic specification of dynamics as an informational basis for person and action perception: Expectation, gender recognition, and deceptive intention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 585-615.

Runeson, S (1994). Perception of biological motion: The KSD-principle. In Perceiving Events and Objects, G. Jansson, S.S. Bergstrom & W. Epstein, Eds., pp 383-405.

Runeson, S., Juslin, P., & Olsson, H. (2000). Visual perception of dynamic properties: Cue heuristics versus direct-perceptual competence. Psychological Review, 107(3), 525-555.

Schenk, T., & Zihl, J. (1997). Visual motion perception after brain damage .2. Deficits in form-from-motion perception. Neuropsychologia, 35(9), 1299-1310.

Schmidt, R. C., Christianson, N., Carello, C., & Baron, R. (1994). Effects of Social and Physical Variables on between-Person Visual Coordination. Ecological Psychology, 6(3), 159-183.

Schmidt, R. C., O'Brien, B., & Sysko, R. (1999). Self-organization of between-persons cooperative tasks and possible applications to sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 30(4), 558-579.

Schmuckler, M. A., & Fairhall, J. L. (2001). Visual-proprioceptive intermodal perception using point light displays. Child Development, 72(4), 949-962.

Schoner, G. (1991). Dynamic Theory of Action-Perception Patterns - the Moving Room Paradigm. Biological Cybernetics, 64(6), 455-462.

Schoner, G., Zanone, P. G., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1992). Learning as Change of Coordination Dynamics - Theory and Experiment. Journal of Motor Behavior, 24(1), 29-48.

Scully, D., & Carnegie, E. (1998). Observational learning in motor skill acquisition: A look at demonstrations. Irish Journal of Psychology, 19(4), 472-485.

Scully, D. M. (1986). Visual-Perception of Technical Execution and Aesthetic Quality in Biological Motion. Human Movement Science, 5(2), 185-206.

Shiffrar, M. & Freyd, J.J. (1990). Apparent motion of the human body. Psychological Science, 1, 257-264.

Shiffrar, M. & Freyd, J.J. (1993). Timing and apparent motion path choice with human body photographs. Psychological Science, 4, 379-384.

Shiffrar, M., Lichtey, L., & Chatterjee, S. H. (1997). The perception of biological motion across apertures. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(1), 51-59.

Shipley, T. F., & Cohen, L. R. (2000). Affordances for coordinated action in point-light walker displays. Ecological Psychology, 12(1), 87-92.

Song, Y., Goncalves, L., & Di Bernardo, E. (2001). Monocular perception of biological motion in Johansson displays. Computer Vision and Image Understanding, 81(3), 303-327.

Sparrow, W. A., Shinkfield, A. J., Day, R. H., & Zerman, L. (1999). Visual perception of human activity and gender in biological- motion displays by individuals with mental retardation. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 104(3), 215-226.

Stevenage, S. V., Nixon, M. S., & Vince, K. (1999). Visual analysis of gait as a cue to identity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13(6), 513-526.

Stone, J. V. (1998). Object recognition using spatiotemporal signatures. Vision Research, 38(7), 947-951.

Stone, J. V. (1999). Object recognition: view-specificity and motion-specificity. Vision Research, 39(24), 4032-4044.

Sumi, S. (2000). Perception of point-light walker produced by eight lights attached to the back of the walker. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 59(2), 126-132.

Thomas, S. M., & Jordan, T. R. (2001). Techniques for the production of point-light and fully illuminated video displays from identical recordings. Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers, 33(1), 59-64.

Thornton, I. M., Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (1998). The visual perception of human locomotion. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15(6-8), 535-552.

Tomonaga, M. (2001). Visual search for biological motion patterns in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Psychologia, 44(1), 46-59.

Tremoulet, P. D., & Feldman, J. (2000). Perception of animacy from the motion of a single object. Perception, 29(8), 943-951.

Vaina, L. M., Lemay, M., Bienfang, D. C., Choi, A. Y., & Nakayama, K. (1990). Intact Biological Motion and Structure from Motion Perception in a Patient with Impaired Motion Mechanisms - a Case-Study. Visual Neuroscience, 5(4), 353-369.

Vaina, L. M., Solomon, J., Chowdhury, S., Sinha, P., & Belliveau, J. W. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of biological motion perception in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(20), 11656-11661.

Verfaillie, K. (1992). Variant Points-of-View on Viewpoint Invariance. Canadian Journal of Psychology-Revue Canadienne De Psychologie, 46(2), 215-235.

Verfaillie, K. (1993). Orientation-Dependent Priming Effects in the Perception of Biological Motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 19(5), 992-1013.

Verfaillie, K. (1997). Transsaccadic memory for the egocentric and allocentric position of a biological-motion walker. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 23(3), 739-760.

Verfaillie, K. (2000). Perceiving human locomotion: Priming effects in direction discrimination. Brain and Cognition, 44(2), 192-213.

Verfaillie, K., & De Graef, P. (2000). Transsaccadic memory for position and orientation of saccade source and target. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 26(4), 1243-1259.

Verfaillie, K., Detroy, A., & Vanrensbergen, J. (1994). Transsaccadic Integration of Biological Motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 20(3), 649-670.

Viviani, P., BaudBovy, G., & Redolfi, M. (1997). Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: Further evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance, 23(4), 1232-1252.

Walk, R.D. & Homan, C.P. (1984). Emotion and dance in dynamic light displays. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 22, 437-440.

Wheaton, K. J., Pipingas, A., Silberstein, R. B., & Puce, A. (2001). Human neural responses elicited to observing the actions of others. Visual Neuroscience, 18(3), 401-406.

Wilson, M. (2001). Perceiving imitatible stimuli: Consequences of isomorphism between input and output. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 543-553.

 

Top

 

 

Links

A list of links including demonstrations and researchers

A short summary of biological motion by Randolph Blake

 

Top